OK, Jacques just told us we should blog about our work, so I’ll try
and explain what I’m actually doing- or trying to do at least. I recently
joined the St-Pée lab as an assistant lecturer, after a postdoc with Andrew
Hendry and Simon Reader in Montréal. This is where I discovered how fishes and
particularly guppies are a wonderful
model to test some exciting evolutionary concepts. Why guppies? Well, these
fishes are an iconic (and pretty cute) model because they evolve rapidly in
response to different ecological conditions. For instance, there are different
lineages having evolved
with or without predators, and with or without parasites in several Caribbean
rivers, giving us access to totally independent instances of potential evolutionary divergence between
contrasting environments. So they are a wonderful species model to test
the parallel evolution of phenotypic
responses to environmental variations in the wild (and you can go to the Caribbean
islands to study them, which is not too bad either).
Fig
1.
Nice Trinidadian guppies (Picture from Zandona et al. Funct. Ecol 25:5)
The effect of
predators was well characterized in this system. For instance, guppies having
evolved with predators are more tenacious and have a higher pace-of life (meaning
they reproduce sooner and more but die younger) than guppies having evolved
without predators. This makes sense. If you have a high probability of an early
death, you should reproduce as quickly as possible. But a big question remained.
Do parasites have the same kind of
effect or not? It is not obvious. After all, even if parasites can affect
their host fitness by weakening them, they are not supposed to kill them, or
they might die as well. So, do parasites matter and are they able to affect the
evolutionary trajectories of their hosts? Andrew and coworkers set up a nice set of studies
to test these hypotheses on morphological and life-history traits. Below is a
picture of the kind of parasite they choose to focus on: these nasty critters
are called Gyrodactylus, they fix themselves on the skin of fishes using
their hooks, and feed from the mucus of their poor hosts. When the fish is not
juicy anymore, they can jump on another fish through fish-to-fish contact. Lovely,
don’t you think?
Figure 2:
Gyrodactylus ectoparasite. Beware. Photo: Eve Zeyl
As for me, I was particularly interested in how these fascinating little
parasites could affect the evolution of their
host behavior. I knew from my PhD on birds how parasites could enhance host
immune system across generations, but it seemed to me that they should also
affect the evolution of their host behavior. After all, the best way of avoiding
the pathogenic effects of parasites is to avoid encountering them. It may even
spare you the costs of mounting a costly immune response. So I was lucky enough
to convince the Fyssen foundation to fund my project, and this is how I ended
testing how parasites could drive the evolution of personality traits (consistent behavioral traits across contexts)
and syndromes (suites of
interrelated behaviors) in guppies. We focused on boldness (reaction towards
stressful situations), activity and sociability, and the link between those
behaviors (syndromes). Indeed, when you evolve with parasites, maybe you should
be shyer to avoid being exposed to parasites and/or evolve a lower tendency to
join conspecifics since they may infect you. In a nutshell, parasites might shape
the different sets of behaviors you
should display towards new situations
and conspecifics. To test those hypotheses, we compared different guppy
populations having evolved with or without predators and with or without
parasites in two independent replicate rivers in Trinidad. That’s how I ended
up fishing guppies in the wonderful rivers of Trinidad in the Caribs (+30°C) and
testing their behavior in the lab in
Montréal (-30°C) (yes, I let you imagine how hard it was to get off the plane).
Fig
3.
Sampling guppies in the Marianne river (my favorite river). Picture: Felipe
Perez.
To be honest, I expected behavior to vary greatly and not
necessarily in a predictable manner. But to my surprise, fish displayed consistent
and quite parallel differences in their behaviors between different rivers,
even after two generations raised in the lab. Look at those nice graphs (thank
you Amandine).
Fig 4:
Shyness level (measured as the latency to
leave a shelter in seconds) in guppies having evolved in contrasted
evolutionary regimes in two independent rivers (Marianne and Aripo).
On the left is the shyness
level (latency to leave a shelter) of wild-caught guppies (generation F0)
from different evolutionary regimes (having evolved with parasites and
predators, parasites only, or no parasite and no predator- you can’t have sites
with predators only because of the topography of the rivers) within the two
independent rivers (Marianne in white and Aripo in yellow). On the right we
tested again the behavior of the F2 lab-raised generation (the babies of their
babies) to see if behavioral differences stemmed from environmental or
genetic-based factors. Well, you can see some variations between the F0 and the
F2 generation, which might be due to environmental effects or habituation to
captivity across generations. But the differences in behavior between evolutionary regimes remain pretty parallel in the Marianne and the
Aripo rivers, even at the F2 generation.
This suggests that whatever the initial genetic background of the
populations and the environmental variations between the two independent rivers
(those rivers were quite different in a lot of aspects), predators seem to consistently
drive the evolution of a genetic-based bolder behavior (this had been already
shown before). And more importantly, parasites seem to consistently drive the evolution of a shyer behavior in a
parallel way in the two rivers. In other words, parasites seem to repeatably select
for a more cautious behavior, maybe because it can decrease the fish chances to
encounter a parasite, although this adaptive interpretation remain to be
tested. We also tested other behaviors and the links between them, and saw that
activity is also affected, as well as shoaling, but in a more complex way. In
addition, predators and parasites seemed to decouple the correlations between traits, that is to say, the structure of
syndromes. This suggests that predators and parasites not only affect the mean
value of behavioral traits, but also impact the whole structure of behavioral packages that individuals
can display.
This is pretty exciting, and supports the hypothesis that selection
act on suites of behavioral traits as a whole rather than on isolated traits,
and that parasites might be a more potent evolutionary force than previously
thought. And more importantly, this helps us realizing how parasites can have a
central role in shaping the diversity of life. So, next time you see a
parasite, think about it. It might hurt you a little bit, but it might also
affect the evolution of your gran-gran children. Pretty scary…
Author: Lisa Jacquin
More info: https://sites.google.com/site/jacquinlisa/
References
Jacquin
L, Reader S,
Matelunna J, Patalas I, Perez-Jvostov F, Hendry A. Variations in
behavioral syndromes across parasitism and predation regimes in wild
Trinidadian guppies. In prep for Behavioral
Ecology
Gotanda,
K.M., Delaire, L.C., Raeymaekers, J.A.M., Pérez-Jvostov, F., Dargent, F.,
Bentzen, P., Scott, M.E., Fussmann, G.F., Hendry, A.P., 2012. Adding parasites
to the guppy-predation story: insights from field surveys. Oecologia 172,
155–166.
Barber,
I., Dingemanse, N.J., 2010. Parasitism and the evolutionary ecology of animal
personality. Philosophical Transactions of the Royal Society B: Biological
Sciences 365, 4077–4088.
Aucun commentaire:
Enregistrer un commentaire